Perception of vocoded speech in domestic dogs

Abstract Humans have an impressive ability to comprehend signal-degraded speech; however, the extent to which comprehension of degraded speech relies on human-specific features of speech perception vs. more general cognitive processes is unknown. Since dogs live alongside humans and regularly hear s...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Main Authors: Amritha Mallikarjun, Emily Shroads, Rochelle S. Newman
Format: Article
Language:English
Published: Springer 2024-04-01
Series:Animal Cognition
Subjects:
Online Access:https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-024-01869-3
Tags: Add Tag
No Tags, Be the first to tag this record!
_version_ 1832585588338851840
author Amritha Mallikarjun
Emily Shroads
Rochelle S. Newman
author_facet Amritha Mallikarjun
Emily Shroads
Rochelle S. Newman
author_sort Amritha Mallikarjun
collection DOAJ
description Abstract Humans have an impressive ability to comprehend signal-degraded speech; however, the extent to which comprehension of degraded speech relies on human-specific features of speech perception vs. more general cognitive processes is unknown. Since dogs live alongside humans and regularly hear speech, they can be used as a model to differentiate between these possibilities. One often-studied type of degraded speech is noise-vocoded speech (sometimes thought of as cochlear-implant-simulation speech). Noise-vocoded speech is made by dividing the speech signal into frequency bands (channels), identifying the amplitude envelope of each individual band, and then using these envelopes to modulate bands of noise centered over the same frequency regions – the result is a signal with preserved temporal cues, but vastly reduced frequency information. Here, we tested dogs’ recognition of familiar words produced in 16-channel vocoded speech. In the first study, dogs heard their names and unfamiliar dogs’ names (foils) in vocoded speech as well as natural speech. In the second study, dogs heard 16-channel vocoded speech only. Dogs listened longer to their vocoded name than vocoded foils in both experiments, showing that they can comprehend a 16-channel vocoded version of their name without prior exposure to vocoded speech, and without immediate exposure to the natural-speech version of their name. Dogs’ name recognition in the second study was mediated by the number of phonemes in the dogs’ name, suggesting that phonological context plays a role in degraded speech comprehension.
format Article
id doaj-art-b07273cbe1bc407697816c8dff5ec994
institution Kabale University
issn 1435-9456
language English
publishDate 2024-04-01
publisher Springer
record_format Article
series Animal Cognition
spelling doaj-art-b07273cbe1bc407697816c8dff5ec9942025-01-26T12:43:44ZengSpringerAnimal Cognition1435-94562024-04-0127111310.1007/s10071-024-01869-3Perception of vocoded speech in domestic dogsAmritha Mallikarjun0Emily Shroads1Rochelle S. Newman2Penn Vet Working Dog Center, University of Pennsylvania School of Veterinary MedicineDepartment of Hearing and Speech Sciences, University of MarylandDepartment of Hearing and Speech Sciences, University of MarylandAbstract Humans have an impressive ability to comprehend signal-degraded speech; however, the extent to which comprehension of degraded speech relies on human-specific features of speech perception vs. more general cognitive processes is unknown. Since dogs live alongside humans and regularly hear speech, they can be used as a model to differentiate between these possibilities. One often-studied type of degraded speech is noise-vocoded speech (sometimes thought of as cochlear-implant-simulation speech). Noise-vocoded speech is made by dividing the speech signal into frequency bands (channels), identifying the amplitude envelope of each individual band, and then using these envelopes to modulate bands of noise centered over the same frequency regions – the result is a signal with preserved temporal cues, but vastly reduced frequency information. Here, we tested dogs’ recognition of familiar words produced in 16-channel vocoded speech. In the first study, dogs heard their names and unfamiliar dogs’ names (foils) in vocoded speech as well as natural speech. In the second study, dogs heard 16-channel vocoded speech only. Dogs listened longer to their vocoded name than vocoded foils in both experiments, showing that they can comprehend a 16-channel vocoded version of their name without prior exposure to vocoded speech, and without immediate exposure to the natural-speech version of their name. Dogs’ name recognition in the second study was mediated by the number of phonemes in the dogs’ name, suggesting that phonological context plays a role in degraded speech comprehension.https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-024-01869-3Canine speech perceptionDegraded speech perceptionVocoded speech
spellingShingle Amritha Mallikarjun
Emily Shroads
Rochelle S. Newman
Perception of vocoded speech in domestic dogs
Animal Cognition
Canine speech perception
Degraded speech perception
Vocoded speech
title Perception of vocoded speech in domestic dogs
title_full Perception of vocoded speech in domestic dogs
title_fullStr Perception of vocoded speech in domestic dogs
title_full_unstemmed Perception of vocoded speech in domestic dogs
title_short Perception of vocoded speech in domestic dogs
title_sort perception of vocoded speech in domestic dogs
topic Canine speech perception
Degraded speech perception
Vocoded speech
url https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-024-01869-3
work_keys_str_mv AT amrithamallikarjun perceptionofvocodedspeechindomesticdogs
AT emilyshroads perceptionofvocodedspeechindomesticdogs
AT rochellesnewman perceptionofvocodedspeechindomesticdogs