Rats and mice rapidly update timed behaviors

Abstract Keeping track of time intervals is a crucial aspect of behavior and cognition. Many theoretical models of how the brain times behavior make predictions for steady-state performance of well-learned intervals, but the rate of learning intervals in these models varies greatly, ranging from one...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Main Authors: N. Aggadi, S. Krikawa, T. A. Paine, P. Simen, C. D. Howard
Format: Article
Language:English
Published: Springer 2025-01-01
Series:Animal Cognition
Subjects:
Online Access:https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-025-01930-9
Tags: Add Tag
No Tags, Be the first to tag this record!
_version_ 1832585571767156736
author N. Aggadi
S. Krikawa
T. A. Paine
P. Simen
C. D. Howard
author_facet N. Aggadi
S. Krikawa
T. A. Paine
P. Simen
C. D. Howard
author_sort N. Aggadi
collection DOAJ
description Abstract Keeping track of time intervals is a crucial aspect of behavior and cognition. Many theoretical models of how the brain times behavior make predictions for steady-state performance of well-learned intervals, but the rate of learning intervals in these models varies greatly, ranging from one-shot learning to learning over thousands of trials. Here, we explored how quickly rats and mice adapt to changes in interval durations using a serial fixed-interval task. In the first experiment, animals experienced randomly selected fixed-intervals of 12, 24, 36, 48, or 60 s, for blocks ranging from 13 to 21 trials. Consistent with previous work, animals abruptly increased lever pressing as reward availability approached, and these ‘start times’ scaled with the interval duration for both species. We then quantified the rate of updating to new trial durations and found that rodents consistently updated their start times within 2–3 trials following a change in interval duration, before stabilizing their behavior by the third or fourth trial. To account for repeated exposures to fixed-interval durations, a second set of animals was tested with new fixed-intervals after being trained on the serial fixed-interval task described above. Next, a third group was trained on fixed-interval durations that were generated de novo in each day. In each of these contexts, rodents rapidly increased or decreased their start times to mirror new FI durations following exposure to 1–2 trials of new intervals following block transitions. This work adds to growing evidence for rapid duration learning across species, highlighting the need for timing models to be capable of rapid updating in dynamic temporal scenarios.
format Article
id doaj-art-6fa1241c561e499b9b47153b9a706c6e
institution Kabale University
issn 1435-9456
language English
publishDate 2025-01-01
publisher Springer
record_format Article
series Animal Cognition
spelling doaj-art-6fa1241c561e499b9b47153b9a706c6e2025-01-26T12:43:40ZengSpringerAnimal Cognition1435-94562025-01-0128111310.1007/s10071-025-01930-9Rats and mice rapidly update timed behaviorsN. Aggadi0S. Krikawa1T. A. Paine2P. Simen3C. D. Howard4Neuroscience Department, Oberlin CollegeNeuroscience Department, Oberlin CollegeNeuroscience Department, Oberlin CollegeNeuroscience Department, Oberlin CollegeNeuroscience Department, Oberlin CollegeAbstract Keeping track of time intervals is a crucial aspect of behavior and cognition. Many theoretical models of how the brain times behavior make predictions for steady-state performance of well-learned intervals, but the rate of learning intervals in these models varies greatly, ranging from one-shot learning to learning over thousands of trials. Here, we explored how quickly rats and mice adapt to changes in interval durations using a serial fixed-interval task. In the first experiment, animals experienced randomly selected fixed-intervals of 12, 24, 36, 48, or 60 s, for blocks ranging from 13 to 21 trials. Consistent with previous work, animals abruptly increased lever pressing as reward availability approached, and these ‘start times’ scaled with the interval duration for both species. We then quantified the rate of updating to new trial durations and found that rodents consistently updated their start times within 2–3 trials following a change in interval duration, before stabilizing their behavior by the third or fourth trial. To account for repeated exposures to fixed-interval durations, a second set of animals was tested with new fixed-intervals after being trained on the serial fixed-interval task described above. Next, a third group was trained on fixed-interval durations that were generated de novo in each day. In each of these contexts, rodents rapidly increased or decreased their start times to mirror new FI durations following exposure to 1–2 trials of new intervals following block transitions. This work adds to growing evidence for rapid duration learning across species, highlighting the need for timing models to be capable of rapid updating in dynamic temporal scenarios.https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-025-01930-9Interval timingLearningDynamic timing
spellingShingle N. Aggadi
S. Krikawa
T. A. Paine
P. Simen
C. D. Howard
Rats and mice rapidly update timed behaviors
Animal Cognition
Interval timing
Learning
Dynamic timing
title Rats and mice rapidly update timed behaviors
title_full Rats and mice rapidly update timed behaviors
title_fullStr Rats and mice rapidly update timed behaviors
title_full_unstemmed Rats and mice rapidly update timed behaviors
title_short Rats and mice rapidly update timed behaviors
title_sort rats and mice rapidly update timed behaviors
topic Interval timing
Learning
Dynamic timing
url https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-025-01930-9
work_keys_str_mv AT naggadi ratsandmicerapidlyupdatetimedbehaviors
AT skrikawa ratsandmicerapidlyupdatetimedbehaviors
AT tapaine ratsandmicerapidlyupdatetimedbehaviors
AT psimen ratsandmicerapidlyupdatetimedbehaviors
AT cdhoward ratsandmicerapidlyupdatetimedbehaviors